Speech perception
Part of a series on | ||||||
Phonetics | ||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Part of the Linguistics Series | ||||||
Subdisciplines | ||||||
Articulation | ||||||
|
||||||
Acoustics | ||||||
|
||||||
Perception | ||||||
|
||||||
Linguistics portal | ||||||
Speech perception is the process by which the sounds of language are heard, interpreted, and understood. The study of speech perception is closely linked to the fields of phonology and phonetics in linguistics and cognitive psychology and perception in psychology. Research in speech perception seeks to understand how human listeners recognize speech sounds and use this information to understand spoken language. Speech perception research has applications in building computer systems that can recognize speech, in improving speech recognition for hearing- and language-impaired listeners, and in foreign-language teaching.
The process of perceiving speech begins at the level of the sound signal and the process of audition. (For a complete description of the process of audition see Hearing.) After processing the initial auditory signal, speech sounds are further processed to extract acoustic cues and phonetic information. This speech information can then be used for higher-level language processes, such as word recognition.
Acoustic cues
[edit]Acoustic cues are sensory cues contained in the speech sound signal which are used in speech perception to differentiate speech sounds belonging to different phonetic categories. For example, one of the most studied cues in speech is voice onset time or VOT. VOT is a primary cue signaling the difference between voiced and voiceless plosives, such as "b" and "p". Other cues differentiate sounds that are produced at different places of articulation or manners of articulation. The speech system must also combine these cues to determine the category of a specific speech sound. This is often thought of in terms of abstract representations of phonemes. These representations can then be combined for use in word recognition and other language processes.
It is not easy to identify what acoustic cues listeners are sensitive to when perceiving a particular speech sound:
At first glance, the solution to the problem of how we perceive speech seems deceptively simple. If one could identify stretches of the acoustic waveform that correspond to units of perception, then the path from sound to meaning would be clear. However, this correspondence or mapping has proven extremely difficult to find, even after some forty-five years of research on the problem.[1]
If a specific aspect of the acoustic waveform indicated one linguistic unit, a series of tests using speech synthesizers would be sufficient to determine such a cue or cues. However, there are two significant obstacles:
- One acoustic aspect of the speech signal may cue different linguistically relevant dimensions. For example, the duration of a vowel in English can indicate whether or not the vowel is stressed, or whether it is in a syllable closed by a voiced or a voiceless consonant, and in some cases (like American English /ɛ/ and /æ/) it can distinguish the identity of vowels.[2] Some experts even argue that duration can help in distinguishing of what is traditionally called short and long vowels in English.[3]
- One linguistic unit can be cued by several acoustic properties. For example, in a classic experiment, Alvin Liberman (1957) showed that the onset formant transitions of /d/ differ depending on the following vowel (see Figure 1) but they are all interpreted as the phoneme /d/ by listeners.[4]
Linearity and the segmentation problem
[edit]Although listeners perceive speech as a stream of discrete units[citation needed] (phonemes, syllables, and words), this linearity is difficult to see in the physical speech signal (see Figure 2 for an example). Speech sounds do not strictly follow one another, rather, they overlap.[5] A speech sound is influenced by the ones that precede and the ones that follow. This influence can even be exerted at a distance of two or more segments (and across syllable- and word-boundaries).[5]
Because the speech signal is not linear, there is a problem of segmentation. It is difficult to delimit a stretch of speech signal as belonging to a single perceptual unit. As an example, the acoustic properties of the phoneme /d/ will depend on the production of the following vowel (because of coarticulation).
Lack of invariance
[edit]The research and application of speech perception must deal with several problems which result from what has been termed the lack of invariance. Reliable constant relations between a phoneme of a language and its acoustic manifestation in speech are difficult to find. There are several reasons for this:
Context-induced variation
[edit]Phonetic environment affects the acoustic properties of speech sounds. For example, /u/ in English is fronted when surrounded by coronal consonants.[6] Or, the voice onset time marking the boundary between voiced and voiceless plosives are different for labial, alveolar and velar plosives and they shift under stress or depending on the position within a syllable.[7]
Variation due to differing speech conditions
[edit]One important factor that causes variation is differing speech rate. Many phonemic contrasts are constituted by temporal characteristics (short vs. long vowels or consonants, affricates vs. fricatives, plosives vs. glides, voiced vs. voiceless plosives, etc.) and they are certainly affected by changes in speaking tempo.[1] Another major source of variation is articulatory carefulness vs. sloppiness which is typical for connected speech (articulatory "undershoot" is obviously reflected in the acoustic properties of the sounds produced).
Variation due to different speaker identity
[edit]The resulting acoustic structure of concrete speech productions depends on the physical and psychological properties of individual speakers. Men, women, and children generally produce voices having different pitch. Because speakers have vocal tracts of different sizes (due to sex and age especially) the resonant frequencies (formants), which are important for recognition of speech sounds, will vary in their absolute values across individuals[8] (see Figure 3 for an illustration of this). Research shows that infants at the age of 7.5 months cannot recognize information presented by speakers of different genders; however by the age of 10.5 months, they can detect the similarities.[9] Dialect and foreign accent can also cause variation, as can the social characteristics of the speaker and listener.[10]
Perceptual constancy and normalization
[edit]Despite the great variety of different speakers and different conditions, listeners perceive vowels and consonants as constant categories. It has been proposed that this is achieved by means of the perceptual normalization process in which listeners filter out the noise (i.e. variation) to arrive at the underlying category. Vocal-tract-size differences result in formant-frequency variation across speakers; therefore a listener has to adjust his/her perceptual system to the acoustic characteristics of a particular speaker. This may be accomplished by considering the ratios of formants rather than their absolute values.[11][12][13] This process has been called vocal tract normalization (see Figure 3 for an example). Similarly, listeners are believed to adjust the perception of duration to the current tempo of the speech they are listening to – this has been referred to as speech rate normalization.
Whether or not normalization actually takes place and what is its exact nature is a matter of theoretical controversy (see theories below). Perceptual constancy is a phenomenon not specific to speech perception only; it exists in other types of perception too.
Categorical perception
[edit]Categorical perception is involved in processes of perceptual differentiation. People perceive speech sounds categorically, that is to say, they are more likely to notice the differences between categories (phonemes) than within categories. The perceptual space between categories is therefore warped, the centers of categories (or "prototypes") working like a sieve[14] or like magnets[15] for incoming speech sounds.
In an artificial continuum between a voiceless and a voiced bilabial plosive, each new step differs from the preceding one in the amount of VOT. The first sound is a pre-voiced [b], i.e. it has a negative VOT. Then, increasing the VOT, it reaches zero, i.e. the plosive is a plain unaspirated voiceless [p]. Gradually, adding the same amount of VOT at a time, the plosive is eventually a strongly aspirated voiceless bilabial [pʰ]. (Such a continuum was used in an experiment by Lisker and Abramson in 1970.[16] The sounds they used are available online.) In this continuum of, for example, seven sounds, native English listeners will identify the first three sounds as /b/ and the last three sounds as /p/ with a clear boundary between the two categories.[16] A two-alternative identification (or categorization) test will yield a discontinuous categorization function (see red curve in Figure 4).
In tests of the ability to discriminate between two sounds with varying VOT values but having a constant VOT distance from each other (20 ms for instance), listeners are likely to perform at chance level if both sounds fall within the same category and at nearly 100% level if each sound falls in a different category (see the blue discrimination curve in Figure 4).
The conclusion to make from both the identification and the discrimination test is that listeners will have different sensitivity to the same relative increase in VOT depending on whether or not the boundary between categories was crossed. Similar perceptual adjustment is attested for other acoustic cues as well.
Top-down influences
[edit]In a classic experiment, Richard M. Warren (1970) replaced one phoneme of a word with a cough-like sound. Perceptually, his subjects restored the missing speech sound without any difficulty and could not accurately identify which phoneme had been disturbed,[17] a phenomenon known as the phonemic restoration effect. Therefore, the process of speech perception is not necessarily uni-directional.
Another basic experiment compared recognition of naturally spoken words within a phrase versus the same words in isolation, finding that perception accuracy usually drops in the latter condition. To probe the influence of semantic knowledge on perception, Garnes and Bond (1976) similarly used carrier sentences where target words only differed in a single phoneme (bay/day/gay, for example) whose quality changed along a continuum. When put into different sentences that each naturally led to one interpretation, listeners tended to judge ambiguous words according to the meaning of the whole sentence[18] .[19] That is, higher-level language processes connected with morphology, syntax, or semantics may interact with basic speech perception processes to aid in recognition of speech sounds.
It may be the case that it is not necessary and maybe even not possible for a listener to recognize phonemes before recognizing higher units, like words for example. After obtaining at least a fundamental piece of information about phonemic structure of the perceived entity from the acoustic signal, listeners can compensate for missing or noise-masked phonemes using their knowledge of the spoken language. Compensatory mechanisms might even operate at the sentence level such as in learned songs, phrases and verses, an effect backed-up by neural coding patterns consistent with the missed continuous speech fragments,[20] despite the lack of all relevant bottom-up sensory input.
Acquired language impairment
[edit]The first ever hypothesis of speech perception was used with patients who acquired an auditory comprehension deficit, also known as receptive aphasia. Since then there have been many disabilities that have been classified, which resulted in a true definition of "speech perception".[21] The term 'speech perception' describes the process of interest that employs sub lexical contexts to the probe process. It consists of many different language and grammatical functions, such as: features, segments (phonemes), syllabic structure (unit of pronunciation), phonological word forms (how sounds are grouped together), grammatical features, morphemic (prefixes and suffixes), and semantic information (the meaning of the words). In the early years, they were more interested in the acoustics of speech. For instance, they were looking at the differences between /ba/ or /da/, but now research has been directed to the response in the brain from the stimuli. In recent years, there has been a model developed to create a sense of how speech perception works; this model is known as the dual stream model. This model has drastically changed from how psychologists look at perception. The first section of the dual stream model is the ventral pathway. This pathway incorporates middle temporal gyrus, inferior temporal sulcus and perhaps the inferior temporal gyrus. The ventral pathway shows phonological representations to the lexical or conceptual representations, which is the meaning of the words. The second section of the dual stream model is the dorsal pathway. This pathway includes the sylvian parietotemporal, inferior frontal gyrus, anterior insula, and premotor cortex. Its primary function is to take the sensory or phonological stimuli and transfer it into an articulatory-motor representation (formation of speech).[22]
Aphasia
[edit]Aphasia is an impairment of language processing caused by damage to the brain. Different parts of language processing are impacted depending on the area of the brain that is damaged, and aphasia is further classified based on the location of injury or constellation of symptoms. Damage to Broca's area of the brain often results in expressive aphasia which manifests as impairment in speech production. Damage to Wernicke's area often results in receptive aphasia where speech processing is impaired.[23]
Aphasia with impaired speech perception typically shows lesions or damage located in the left temporal or parietal lobes. Lexical and semantic difficulties are common, and comprehension may be affected.[23]
Agnosia
[edit]Agnosia is "the loss or diminution of the ability to recognize familiar objects or stimuli usually as a result of brain damage".[24] There are several different kinds of agnosia that affect every one of our senses, but the two most common related to speech are speech agnosia and phonagnosia.
Speech agnosia: Pure word deafness, or speech agnosia, is an impairment in which a person maintains the ability to hear, produce speech, and even read speech, yet they are unable to understand or properly perceive speech. These patients seem to have all of the skills necessary in order to properly process speech, yet they appear to have no experience associated with speech stimuli. Patients have reported, "I can hear you talking, but I can't translate it".[25] Even though they are physically receiving and processing the stimuli of speech, without the ability to determine the meaning of the speech, they essentially are unable to perceive the speech at all. There are no known treatments that have been found, but from case studies and experiments it is known that speech agnosia is related to lesions in the left hemisphere or both, specifically right temporoparietal dysfunctions.[26]
Phonagnosia: Phonagnosia is associated with the inability to recognize any familiar voices. In these cases, speech stimuli can be heard and even understood but the association of the speech to a certain voice is lost. This can be due to "abnormal processing of complex vocal properties (timbre, articulation, and prosody—elements that distinguish an individual voice".[27] There is no known treatment; however, there is a case report of an epileptic woman who began to experience phonagnosia along with other impairments. Her EEG and MRI results showed "a right cortical parietal T2-hyperintense lesion without gadolinium enhancement and with discrete impairment of water molecule diffusion".[27] So although no treatment has been discovered, phonagnosia can be correlated to postictal parietal cortical dysfunction.
Infant speech perception
[edit]Infants begin the process of language acquisition by being able to detect very small differences between speech sounds. They can discriminate all possible speech contrasts (phonemes). Gradually, as they are exposed to their native language, their perception becomes language-specific, i.e. they learn how to ignore the differences within phonemic categories of the language (differences that may well be contrastive in other languages – for example, English distinguishes two voicing categories of plosives, whereas Thai has three categories; infants must learn which differences are distinctive in their native language uses, and which are not). As infants learn how to sort incoming speech sounds into categories, ignoring irrelevant differences and reinforcing the contrastive ones, their perception becomes categorical. Infants learn to contrast different vowel phonemes of their native language by approximately 6 months of age. The native consonantal contrasts are acquired by 11 or 12 months of age.[28] Some researchers have proposed that infants may be able to learn the sound categories of their native language through passive listening, using a process called statistical learning. Others even claim that certain sound categories are innate, that is, they are genetically specified (see discussion about innate vs. acquired categorical distinctiveness).
If day-old babies are presented with their mother's voice speaking normally, abnormally (in monotone), and a stranger's voice, they react only to their mother's voice speaking normally. When a human and a non-human sound is played, babies turn their head only to the source of human sound. It has been suggested that auditory learning begins already in the pre-natal period.[29]
One of the techniques used to examine how infants perceive speech, besides the head-turn procedure mentioned above, is measuring their sucking rate. In such an experiment, a baby is sucking a special nipple while presented with sounds. First, the baby's normal sucking rate is established. Then a stimulus is played repeatedly. When the baby hears the stimulus for the first time the sucking rate increases but as the baby becomes habituated to the stimulation the sucking rate decreases and levels off. Then, a new stimulus is played to the baby. If the baby perceives the newly introduced stimulus as different from the background stimulus the sucking rate will show an increase.[29] The sucking-rate and the head-turn method are some of the more traditional, behavioral methods for studying speech perception. Among the new methods (see Research methods below) that help us to study speech perception, near-infrared spectroscopy is widely used in infants.[28]
It has also been discovered that even though infants' ability to distinguish between the different phonetic properties of various languages begins to decline around the age of nine months, it is possible to reverse this process by exposing them to a new language in a sufficient way. In a research study by Patricia K. Kuhl, Feng-Ming Tsao, and Huei-Mei Liu, it was discovered that if infants are spoken to and interacted with by a native speaker of Mandarin Chinese, they can actually be conditioned to retain their ability to distinguish different speech sounds within Mandarin that are very different from speech sounds found within the English language. Thus proving that given the right conditions, it is possible to prevent infants' loss of the ability to distinguish speech sounds in languages other than those found in the native language.[30]
Cross-language and second-language
[edit]A large amount of research has studied how users of a language perceive foreign speech (referred to as cross-language speech perception) or second-language speech (second-language speech perception). The latter falls within the domain of second language acquisition.
Languages differ in their phonemic inventories. Naturally, this creates difficulties when a foreign language is encountered. For example, if two foreign-language sounds are assimilated to a single mother-tongue category the difference between them will be very difficult to discern. A classic example of this situation is the observation that Japanese learners of English will have problems with identifying or distinguishing English liquid consonants /l/ and /r/ (see Perception of English /r/ and /l/ by Japanese speakers).[31]
Best (1995) proposed a Perceptual Assimilation Model which describes possible cross-language category assimilation patterns and predicts their consequences.[32] Flege (1995) formulated a Speech Learning Model which combines several hypotheses about second-language (L2) speech acquisition and which predicts, in simple words, that an L2 sound that is not too similar to a native-language (L1) sound will be easier to acquire than an L2 sound that is relatively similar to an L1 sound (because it will be perceived as more obviously "different" by the learner).[33]
In language or hearing impairment
[edit]Research in how people with language or hearing impairment perceive speech is not only intended to discover possible treatments. It can provide insight into the principles underlying non-impaired speech perception.[34] Two areas of research can serve as an example:
Listeners with aphasia
[edit]Aphasia affects both the expression and reception of language. Both two most common types, expressive aphasia and receptive aphasia, affect speech perception to some extent. Expressive aphasia causes moderate difficulties for language understanding. The effect of receptive aphasia on understanding is much more severe. It is agreed upon, that aphasics suffer from perceptual deficits. They usually cannot fully distinguish place of articulation and voicing.[35] As for other features, the difficulties vary. It has not yet been proven whether low-level speech-perception skills are affected in aphasia sufferers or whether their difficulties are caused by higher-level impairment alone.[35]
Listeners with cochlear implants
[edit]Cochlear implantation restores access to the acoustic signal in individuals with sensorineural hearing loss. The acoustic information conveyed by an implant is usually sufficient for implant users to properly recognize speech of people they know even without visual clues.[36] For cochlear implant users, it is more difficult to understand unknown speakers and sounds. The perceptual abilities of children that received an implant after the age of two are significantly better than of those who were implanted in adulthood. A number of factors have been shown to influence perceptual performance, specifically: duration of deafness prior to implantation, age of onset of deafness, age at implantation (such age effects may be related to the Critical period hypothesis) and the duration of using an implant. There are differences between children with congenital and acquired deafness. Postlingually deaf children have better results than the prelingually deaf and adapt to a cochlear implant faster.[36] In both children with cochlear implants and normal hearing, vowels and voice onset time becomes prevalent in development before the ability to discriminate the place of articulation. Several months following implantation, children with cochlear implants can normalize speech perception.
Noise
[edit]One of the fundamental problems in the study of speech is how to deal with noise. This is shown by the difficulty in recognizing human speech that computer recognition systems have. While they can do well at recognizing speech if trained on a specific speaker's voice and under quiet conditions, these systems often do poorly in more realistic listening situations where humans would understand speech with relative ease. To emulate processing patterns that would be held in the brain under normal conditions, prior knowledge is a key neural factor, since a robust learning history may to an extent override the extreme masking effects involved in the complete absence of continuous speech signals.[20]
Music-language connection
[edit]Research into the relationship between music and cognition is an emerging field related to the study of speech perception. Originally it was theorized that the neural signals for music were processed in a specialized "module" in the right hemisphere of the brain. Conversely, the neural signals for language were to be processed by a similar "module" in the left hemisphere.[37] However, utilizing technologies such as fMRI machines, research has shown that two regions of the brain traditionally considered exclusively to process speech, Broca's and Wernicke's areas, also become active during musical activities such as listening to a sequence of musical chords.[37] Other studies, such as one performed by Marques et al. in 2006 showed that 8-year-olds who were given six months of musical training showed an increase in both their pitch detection performance and their electrophysiological measures when made to listen to an unknown foreign language.[38]
Conversely, some research has revealed that, rather than music affecting our perception of speech, our native speech can affect our perception of music. One example is the tritone paradox. The tritone paradox is where a listener is presented with two computer-generated tones (such as C and F-Sharp) that are half an octave (or a tritone) apart and are then asked to determine whether the pitch of the sequence is descending or ascending. One such study, performed by Ms. Diana Deutsch, found that the listener's interpretation of ascending or descending pitch was influenced by the listener's language or dialect, showing variation between those raised in the south of England and those in California or from those in Vietnam and those in California whose native language was English.[37] A second study, performed in 2006 on a group of English speakers and 3 groups of East Asian students at University of Southern California, discovered that English speakers who had begun musical training at or before age 5 had an 8% chance of having perfect pitch.[37]
Speech phenomenology
[edit]The experience of speech
[edit]Casey O'Callaghan, in his article Experiencing Speech, analyzes whether "the perceptual experience of listening to speech differs in phenomenal character"[39] with regards to understanding the language being heard. He argues that an individual's experience when hearing a language they comprehend, as opposed to their experience when hearing a language they have no knowledge of, displays a difference in phenomenal features which he defines as "aspects of what an experience is like"[39] for an individual.
If a subject who is a monolingual native English speaker is presented with a stimulus of speech in German, the string of phonemes will appear as mere sounds and will produce a very different experience than if exactly the same stimulus was presented to a subject who speaks German.
He also examines how speech perception changes when one learning a language. If a subject with no knowledge of the Japanese language was presented with a stimulus of Japanese speech, and then was given the exact same stimuli after being taught Japanese, this same individual would have an extremely different experience.
Research methods
[edit]The methods used in speech perception research can be roughly divided into three groups: behavioral, computational, and, more recently, neurophysiological methods.
Behavioral methods
[edit]Behavioral experiments are based on an active role of a participant, i.e. subjects are presented with stimuli and asked to make conscious decisions about them. This can take the form of an identification test, a discrimination test, similarity rating, etc. These types of experiments help to provide a basic description of how listeners perceive and categorize speech sounds.
Sinewave Speech
[edit]Speech perception has also been analyzed through sinewave speech, a form of synthetic speech where the human voice is replaced by sine waves that mimic the frequencies and amplitudes present in the original speech. When subjects are first presented with this speech, the sinewave speech is interpreted as random noises. But when the subjects are informed that the stimuli actually is speech and are told what is being said, "a distinctive, nearly immediate shift occurs"[39] to how the sinewave speech is perceived.
Computational methods
[edit]Computational modeling has also been used to simulate how speech may be processed by the brain to produce behaviors that are observed. Computer models have been used to address several questions in speech perception, including how the sound signal itself is processed to extract the acoustic cues used in speech, and how speech information is used for higher-level processes, such as word recognition.[40]
Neurophysiological methods
[edit]Neurophysiological methods rely on utilizing information stemming from more direct and not necessarily conscious (pre-attentative) processes. Subjects are presented with speech stimuli in different types of tasks and the responses of the brain are measured. The brain itself can be more sensitive than it appears to be through behavioral responses. For example, the subject may not show sensitivity to the difference between two speech sounds in a discrimination test, but brain responses may reveal sensitivity to these differences.[28] Methods used to measure neural responses to speech include event-related potentials, magnetoencephalography, and near infrared spectroscopy. One important response used with event-related potentials is the mismatch negativity, which occurs when speech stimuli are acoustically different from a stimulus that the subject heard previously.
Neurophysiological methods were introduced into speech perception research for several reasons:
Behavioral responses may reflect late, conscious processes and be affected by other systems such as orthography, and thus they may mask speaker's ability to recognize sounds based on lower-level acoustic distributions.[41]
Without the necessity of taking an active part in the test, even infants can be tested; this feature is crucial in research into acquisition processes. The possibility to observe low-level auditory processes independently from the higher-level ones makes it possible to address long-standing theoretical issues such as whether or not humans possess a specialized module for perceiving speech[42][43] or whether or not some complex acoustic invariance (see lack of invariance above) underlies the recognition of a speech sound.[44]
Theories
[edit]Motor theory
[edit]Some of the earliest work in the study of how humans perceive speech sounds was conducted by Alvin Liberman and his colleagues at Haskins Laboratories.[45] Using a speech synthesizer, they constructed speech sounds that varied in place of articulation along a continuum from /bɑ/ to /dɑ/ to /ɡɑ/. Listeners were asked to identify which sound they heard and to discriminate between two different sounds. The results of the experiment showed that listeners grouped sounds into discrete categories, even though the sounds they were hearing were varying continuously. Based on these results, they proposed the notion of categorical perception as a mechanism by which humans can identify speech sounds.
More recent research using different tasks and methods suggests that listeners are highly sensitive to acoustic differences within a single phonetic category, contrary to a strict categorical account of speech perception.
To provide a theoretical account of the categorical perception data, Liberman and colleagues[46] worked out the motor theory of speech perception, where "the complicated articulatory encoding was assumed to be decoded in the perception of speech by the same processes that are involved in production"[1] (this is referred to as analysis-by-synthesis). For instance, the English consonant /d/ may vary in its acoustic details across different phonetic contexts (see above), yet all /d/'s as perceived by a listener fall within one category (voiced alveolar plosive) and that is because "linguistic representations are abstract, canonical, phonetic segments or the gestures that underlie these segments".[1] When describing units of perception, Liberman later abandoned articulatory movements and proceeded to the neural commands to the articulators[47] and even later to intended articulatory gestures,[48] thus "the neural representation of the utterance that determines the speaker's production is the distal object the listener perceives".[48] The theory is closely related to the modularity hypothesis, which proposes the existence of a special-purpose module, which is supposed to be innate and probably human-specific.
The theory has been criticized in terms of not being able to "provide an account of just how acoustic signals are translated into intended gestures"[49] by listeners. Furthermore, it is unclear how indexical information (e.g. talker-identity) is encoded/decoded along with linguistically relevant information.
Exemplar theory
[edit]Exemplar models of speech perception differ from the four theories mentioned above which suppose that there is no connection between word- and talker-recognition and that the variation across talkers is "noise" to be filtered out.
The exemplar-based approaches claim listeners store information for both word- and talker-recognition. According to this theory, particular instances of speech sounds are stored in the memory of a listener. In the process of speech perception, the remembered instances of e.g. a syllable stored in the listener's memory are compared with the incoming stimulus so that the stimulus can be categorized. Similarly, when recognizing a talker, all the memory traces of utterances produced by that talker are activated and the talker's identity is determined. Supporting this theory are several experiments reported by Johnson[13] that suggest that our signal identification is more accurate when we are familiar with the talker or when we have visual representation of the talker's gender. When the talker is unpredictable or the sex misidentified, the error rate in word-identification is much higher.
The exemplar models have to face several objections, two of which are (1) insufficient memory capacity to store every utterance ever heard and, concerning the ability to produce what was heard, (2) whether also the talker's own articulatory gestures are stored or computed when producing utterances that would sound as the auditory memories.[13][49]
Acoustic landmarks and distinctive features
[edit]Kenneth N. Stevens proposed acoustic landmarks and distinctive features as a relation between phonological features and auditory properties. According to this view, listeners are inspecting the incoming signal for the so-called acoustic landmarks which are particular events in the spectrum carrying information about gestures which produced them. Since these gestures are limited by the capacities of humans' articulators and listeners are sensitive to their auditory correlates, the lack of invariance simply does not exist in this model. The acoustic properties of the landmarks constitute the basis for establishing the distinctive features. Bundles of them uniquely specify phonetic segments (phonemes, syllables, words).[50]
In this model, the incoming acoustic signal is believed to be first processed to determine the so-called landmarks which are special spectral events in the signal; for example, vowels are typically marked by higher frequency of the first formant, consonants can be specified as discontinuities in the signal and have lower amplitudes in lower and middle regions of the spectrum. These acoustic features result from articulation. In fact, secondary articulatory movements may be used when enhancement of the landmarks is needed due to external conditions such as noise. Stevens claims that coarticulation causes only limited and moreover systematic and thus predictable variation in the signal which the listener is able to deal with. Within this model therefore, what is called the lack of invariance is simply claimed not to exist.
Landmarks are analyzed to determine certain articulatory events (gestures) which are connected with them. In the next stage, acoustic cues are extracted from the signal in the vicinity of the landmarks by means of mental measuring of certain parameters such as frequencies of spectral peaks, amplitudes in low-frequency region, or timing.
The next processing stage comprises acoustic-cues consolidation and derivation of distinctive features. These are binary categories related to articulation (for example [+/- high], [+/- back], [+/- round lips] for vowels; [+/- sonorant], [+/- lateral], or [+/- nasal] for consonants.
Bundles of these features uniquely identify speech segments (phonemes, syllables, words). These segments are part of the lexicon stored in the listener's memory. Its units are activated in the process of lexical access and mapped on the original signal to find out whether they match. If not, another attempt with a different candidate pattern is made. In this iterative fashion, listeners thus reconstruct the articulatory events which were necessary to produce the perceived speech signal. This can be therefore described as analysis-by-synthesis.
This theory thus posits that the distal object of speech perception are the articulatory gestures underlying speech. Listeners make sense of the speech signal by referring to them. The model belongs to those referred to as analysis-by-synthesis.
Fuzzy-logical model
[edit]The fuzzy logical theory of speech perception developed by Dominic Massaro[51] proposes that people remember speech sounds in a probabilistic, or graded, way. It suggests that people remember descriptions of the perceptual units of language, called prototypes. Within each prototype various features may combine. However, features are not just binary (true or false), there is a fuzzy value corresponding to how likely it is that a sound belongs to a particular speech category. Thus, when perceiving a speech signal our decision about what we actually hear is based on the relative goodness of the match between the stimulus information and values of particular prototypes. The final decision is based on multiple features or sources of information, even visual information (this explains the McGurk effect).[49] Computer models of the fuzzy logical theory have been used to demonstrate that the theory's predictions of how speech sounds are categorized correspond to the behavior of human listeners.[52]
Speech mode hypothesis
[edit]Speech mode hypothesis is the idea that the perception of speech requires the use of specialized mental processing.[53][54] The speech mode hypothesis is a branch off of Fodor's modularity theory (see modularity of mind). It utilizes a vertical processing mechanism where limited stimuli are processed by special-purpose areas of the brain that are stimuli specific.[54]
Two versions of speech mode hypothesis:[53]
- Weak version – listening to speech engages previous knowledge of language.
- Strong version – listening to speech engages specialized speech mechanisms for perceiving speech.
Three important experimental paradigms have evolved in the search to find evidence for the speech mode hypothesis. These are dichotic listening, categorical perception, and duplex perception.[53] Through the research in these categories it has been found that there may not be a specific speech mode but instead one for auditory codes that require complicated auditory processing. Also it seems that modularity is learned in perceptual systems.[53] Despite this the evidence and counter-evidence for the speech mode hypothesis is still unclear and needs further research.
Direct realist theory
[edit]The direct realist theory of speech perception (mostly associated with Carol Fowler) is a part of the more general theory of direct realism, which postulates that perception allows us to have direct awareness of the world because it involves direct recovery of the distal source of the event that is perceived. For speech perception, the theory asserts that the objects of perception are actual vocal tract movements, or gestures, and not abstract phonemes or (as in the Motor Theory) events that are causally antecedent to these movements, i.e. intended gestures. Listeners perceive gestures not by means of a specialized decoder (as in the Motor Theory) but because information in the acoustic signal specifies the gestures that form it.[55] By claiming that the actual articulatory gestures that produce different speech sounds are themselves the units of speech perception, the theory bypasses the problem of lack of invariance.
See also
[edit]- Related to the case study of Genie (feral child)
- Neurocomputational speech processing
- Multisensory integration
- Origin of speech
- Speech-Language Pathology
- Motor theory of speech perception
References
[edit]- ^ a b c d Nygaard, L.C., Pisoni, D.B. (1995). "Speech Perception: New Directions in Research and Theory". In J.L. Miller; P.D. Eimas (eds.). Handbook of Perception and Cognition: Speech, Language, and Communication. San Diego: Academic Press.
{{cite encyclopedia}}
: CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link) - ^ Klatt, D.H. (1976). "Linguistic uses of segmental duration in English: Acoustic and perceptual evidence". Journal of the Acoustical Society of America. 59 (5): 1208–1221. Bibcode:1976ASAJ...59.1208K. doi:10.1121/1.380986. PMID 956516.
- ^ Halle, M., Mohanan, K.P. (1985). "Segmental phonology of modern English". Linguistic Inquiry. 16 (1): 57–116.
{{cite journal}}
: CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link) - ^ Liberman, A.M. (1957). "Some results of research on speech perception" (PDF). Journal of the Acoustical Society of America. 29 (1): 117–123. Bibcode:1957ASAJ...29..117L. doi:10.1121/1.1908635. hdl:11858/00-001M-0000-002C-5789-A. Archived from the original (PDF) on 2016-03-03. Retrieved 2007-05-17.
- ^ a b Fowler, C.A. (1995). "Speech production". In J.L. Miller; P.D. Eimas (eds.). Handbook of Perception and Cognition: Speech, Language, and Communication. San Diego: Academic Press.
- ^ Hillenbrand, J.M., Clark, M.J., Nearey, T.M. (2001). "Effects of consonant environment on vowel formant patterns". Journal of the Acoustical Society of America. 109 (2): 748–763. Bibcode:2001ASAJ..109..748H. doi:10.1121/1.1337959. PMID 11248979. S2CID 10751216.
{{cite journal}}
: CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link) - ^ Lisker, L., Abramson, A.S. (1967). "Some effects of context on voice onset time in English plosives" (PDF). Language and Speech. 10 (1): 1–28. doi:10.1177/002383096701000101. PMID 6044530. S2CID 34616732. Archived from the original (PDF) on 2016-03-03. Retrieved 2007-05-17.
{{cite journal}}
: CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link) - ^ a b Hillenbrand, J., Getty, L.A., Clark, M.J., Wheeler, K. (1995). "Acoustic characteristics of American English vowels". Journal of the Acoustical Society of America. 97 (5 Pt 1): 3099–3111. Bibcode:1995ASAJ...97.3099H. doi:10.1121/1.411872. PMID 7759650. S2CID 10104073.
{{cite journal}}
: CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link) - ^ Houston, Derek M.; Juscyk, Peter W. (October 2000). "The role of talker-specific information in word segmentation by infants" (PDF). Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance. 26 (5): 1570–1582. doi:10.1037/0096-1523.26.5.1570. PMID 11039485. Archived from the original (PDF) on 2014-04-30. Retrieved 1 March 2012.
- ^ Hay, Jennifer; Drager, Katie (2010). "Stuffed toys and speech perception". Linguistics. 48 (4): 865–892. doi:10.1515/LING.2010.027. S2CID 143639653.
- ^ a b Syrdal, A.K.; Gopal, H.S. (1986). "A perceptual model of vowel recognition based on the auditory representation of American English vowels". Journal of the Acoustical Society of America. 79 (4): 1086–1100. Bibcode:1986ASAJ...79.1086S. doi:10.1121/1.393381. PMID 3700864.
- ^ Strange, W. (1999). "Perception of vowels: Dynamic constancy". In J.M. Pickett (ed.). The Acoustics of Speech Communication: Fundamentals, Speech Perception Theory, and Technology. Needham Heights (MA): Allyn & Bacon.
- ^ a b c Johnson, K. (2005). "Speaker Normalization in speech perception" (PDF). In Pisoni, D.B.; Remez, R. (eds.). The Handbook of Speech Perception. Oxford: Blackwell Publishers. Retrieved 2007-05-17.
- ^ Trubetzkoy, Nikolay S. (1969). Principles of phonology. Berkeley and Los Angeles: University of California Press. ISBN 978-0-520-01535-7.
- ^ Iverson, P., Kuhl, P.K. (1995). "Mapping the perceptual magnet effect for speech using signal detection theory and multidimensional scaling". Journal of the Acoustical Society of America. 97 (1): 553–562. Bibcode:1995ASAJ...97..553I. doi:10.1121/1.412280. PMID 7860832.
{{cite journal}}
: CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link) - ^ a b Lisker, L., Abramson, A.S. (1970). "The voicing dimension: Some experiments in comparative phonetics" (PDF). Proc. 6th International Congress of Phonetic Sciences. Prague: Academia. pp. 563–567. Archived from the original (PDF) on 2016-03-03. Retrieved 2007-05-17.
{{cite conference}}
: CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link) - ^ Warren, R.M. (1970). "Restoration of missing speech sounds". Science. 167 (3917): 392–393. Bibcode:1970Sci...167..392W. doi:10.1126/science.167.3917.392. PMID 5409744. S2CID 30356740.
- ^ Garnes, S., Bond, Z.S. (1976). "The relationship between acoustic information and semantic expectation". Phonologica 1976. Innsbruck. pp. 285–293.
{{cite conference}}
: CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link) - ^ Jongman A, Wang Y, Kim BH (December 2003). "Contributions of semantic and facial information to perception of nonsibilant fricatives" (PDF). J. Speech Lang. Hear. Res. 46 (6): 1367–77. doi:10.1044/1092-4388(2003/106). hdl:1808/13411. PMID 14700361. Archived from the original (PDF) on 2013-06-14. Retrieved 2017-09-14.
- ^ a b Cervantes Constantino, F; Simon, JZ (2018). "Restoration and Efficiency of the Neural Processing of Continuous Speech Are Promoted by Prior Knowledge". Frontiers in Systems Neuroscience. 12 (56): 56. doi:10.3389/fnsys.2018.00056. PMC 6220042. PMID 30429778.
- ^ Poeppel, David; Monahan, Philip J. (2008). "Speech Perception: Cognitive Foundations and Cortical Implementation". Current Directions in Psychological Science. 17 (2): 80–85. doi:10.1111/j.1467-8721.2008.00553.x. ISSN 0963-7214. S2CID 18628411.
- ^ Hickok G, Poeppel D (May 2007). "The cortical organization of speech processing". Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 8 (5): 393–402. doi:10.1038/nrn2113. PMID 17431404. S2CID 6199399.
- ^ a b Hessler, Dorte; Jonkers, Bastiaanse (December 2010). "The influence of phonetic dimensions on aphasic speech perception". Clinical Linguistics and Phonetics. 12. 24 (12): 980–996. doi:10.3109/02699206.2010.507297. PMID 20887215. S2CID 26478503.
- ^ "Definition of AGNOSIA". www.merriam-webster.com. Retrieved 2017-12-15.
- ^ Howard, Harry (2017). "Welcome to Brain and Language". Welcome to Brain and Language.
- ^ Lambert, J. (1999). "Auditory Agnosia with relative sparing of speech perception". Neurocase. 5 (5): 71–82. doi:10.1016/s0010-9452(89)80007-3. PMID 2707006.
- ^ a b Rocha, Sofia; Amorim, José Manuel; Machado, Álvaro Alexandre; Ferreira, Carla Maria (2015-04-01). "Phonagnosia and Inability to Perceive Time Passage in Right Parietal Lobe Epilepsy". The Journal of Neuropsychiatry and Clinical Neurosciences. 27 (2): e154–e155. doi:10.1176/appi.neuropsych.14040073. ISSN 0895-0172. PMID 25923865.
- ^ a b c Minagawa-Kawai, Y., Mori, K., Naoi, N., Kojima, S. (2006). "Neural Attunement Processes in Infants during the Acquisition of a Language-Specific Phonemic Contrast". The Journal of Neuroscience. 27 (2): 315–321. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1984-06.2007. PMC 6672067. PMID 17215392.
{{cite journal}}
: CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link) - ^ a b Crystal, David (2005). The Cambridge Encyclopedia of Language. Cambridge: CUP. ISBN 978-0-521-55967-6.
- ^ Kuhl, Patricia K.; Feng-Ming Tsao; Huei-Mei Liu (July 2003). "Foreign-language experience in infancy: Effects of short-term exposure and social interaction on phonetic learning". Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences. 100 (15): 9096–9101. Bibcode:2003PNAS..100.9096K. doi:10.1073/pnas.1532872100. PMC 166444. PMID 12861072.
- ^ Iverson, P., Kuhl, P.K., Akahane-Yamada, R., Diesh, E., Thokura, Y., Kettermann, A., Siebert, C. (2003). "A perceptual interference account of acquisition difficulties for non-native phonemes". Cognition. 89 (1): B47–B57. doi:10.1016/S0010-0277(02)00198-1. PMID 12499111. S2CID 463529.
{{cite journal}}
: CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link) - ^ Best, C. T. (1995). "A direct realist view of cross-language speech perception: New Directions in Research and Theory". In Winifred Strange (ed.). Speech perception and linguistic experience: Theoretical and methodological issues. Baltimore: York Press. pp. 171–204.
- ^ Flege, J. (1995). "Second language speech learning: Theory, findings and problems". In Winifred Strange (ed.). Speech perception and linguistic experience: Theoretical and methodological issues. Baltimore: York Press. pp. 233–277.
- ^ Uhler; Yoshinaga-Itano; Gabbard; Rothpletz; Jenkins (March 2011). "infant speech perception in young cochlear implant users". Journal of the American Academy of Audiology. 22 (3): 129–142. doi:10.3766/jaaa.22.3.2. PMID 21545766.
- ^ a b Csépe, V.; Osman-Sagi, J.; Molnar, M.; Gosy, M. (2001). "Impaired speech perception in aphasic patients: event-related potential and neuropsychological assessment". Neuropsychologia. 39 (11): 1194–1208. doi:10.1016/S0028-3932(01)00052-5. PMID 11527557. S2CID 17307242.
- ^ a b Loizou, P. (1998). "Introduction to cochlear implants". IEEE Signal Processing Magazine. 39 (11): 101–130. doi:10.1109/79.708543.
- ^ a b c d Deutsch, Diana; Henthorn, Trevor; Dolson, Mark (Spring 2004). "Speech patterns heard early in life influence later perception of the tritone paradox" (PDF). Music Perception. 21 (3): 357–72. doi:10.1525/mp.2004.21.3.357. Retrieved 29 April 2014.
- ^ Marques, C et al. (2007). Musicians detect pitch violation in foreign language better than nonmusicians: Behavioral and electrophysiological evidence. "Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 19", 1453-1463.
- ^ a b c O'Callaghan, Casey (2010). "Experiencing Speech". Philosophical Issues. 20: 305–327. doi:10.1111/j.1533-6077.2010.00186.x.
- ^ McClelland, J.L. & Elman, J.L. (1986). "The TRACE model of speech perception" (PDF). Cognitive Psychology. 18 (1): 1–86. doi:10.1016/0010-0285(86)90015-0. PMID 3753912. S2CID 7428866. Archived from the original (PDF) on 2007-04-21. Retrieved 2007-05-19.
- ^ Kazanina, N., Phillips, C., Idsardi, W. (2006). "The influence of meaning on the perception of speech sounds" (PDF). PNAS. Vol. 30. pp. 11381–11386. Retrieved 2007-05-19.
{{cite conference}}
: CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link)[permanent dead link] - ^ Gocken, J.M. & Fox R.A. (2001). "Neurological Evidence in Support of a Specialized Phonetic Processing Module". Brain and Language. 78 (2): 241–253. doi:10.1006/brln.2001.2467. PMID 11500073. S2CID 28469116.
- ^ Dehaene-Lambertz, G.; Pallier, C.; Serniclaes, W.; Sprenger-Charolles, L.; Jobert, A.; Dehaene, S. (2005). "Neural correlates of switching from auditory to speech perception" (PDF). NeuroImage. 24 (1): 21–33. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2004.09.039. PMID 15588593. S2CID 11899232. Retrieved 2007-07-04.
- ^ Näätänen, R. (2001). "The perception of speech sounds by the human brain as reflected by the mismatch negativity (MMN) and its magnetic equivalent (MMNm)". Psychophysiology. 38 (1): 1–21. doi:10.1111/1469-8986.3810001. PMID 11321610.
- ^ Liberman, A.M., Harris, K.S., Hoffman, H.S., Griffith, B.C. (1957). "The discrimination of speech sounds within and across phoneme boundaries" (PDF). Journal of Experimental Psychology. 54 (5): 358–368. doi:10.1037/h0044417. PMID 13481283. S2CID 10117886. Retrieved 2007-05-18.
{{cite journal}}
: CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link) - ^ Liberman, A.M., Cooper, F.S., Shankweiler, D.P., & Studdert-Kennedy, M. (1967). "Perception of the speech code" (PDF). Psychological Review. 74 (6): 431–461. doi:10.1037/h0020279. PMID 4170865. Retrieved 2007-05-19.
{{cite journal}}
: CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link) - ^ Liberman, A.M. (1970). "The grammars of speech and language" (PDF). Cognitive Psychology. 1 (4): 301–323. doi:10.1016/0010-0285(70)90018-6. Archived from the original (PDF) on 2015-12-31. Retrieved 2007-07-19.
- ^ a b Liberman, A.M. & Mattingly, I.G. (1985). "The motor theory of speech perception revised" (PDF). Cognition. 21 (1): 1–36. CiteSeerX 10.1.1.330.220. doi:10.1016/0010-0277(85)90021-6. PMID 4075760. S2CID 112932. Archived from the original (PDF) on 2021-04-15. Retrieved 2007-07-19.
- ^ a b c Hayward, Katrina (2000). Experimental Phonetics: An Introduction. Harlow: Longman.
- ^ Stevens, K.N. (2002). "Toward a model of lexical access based on acoustic landmarks and distinctive features" (PDF). Journal of the Acoustical Society of America. 111 (4): 1872–1891. Bibcode:2002ASAJ..111.1872S. doi:10.1121/1.1458026. PMID 12002871. Archived from the original (PDF) on 2007-06-09. Retrieved 2007-05-17.
- ^ Massaro, D.W. (1989). "Testing between the TRACE Model and the Fuzzy Logical Model of Speech perception". Cognitive Psychology. 21 (3): 398–421. doi:10.1016/0010-0285(89)90014-5. PMID 2758786. S2CID 7629786.
- ^ Oden, G.C., Massaro, D.W. (1978). "Integration of featural information in speech perception". Psychological Review. 85 (3): 172–191. doi:10.1037/0033-295X.85.3.172. PMID 663005.
{{cite journal}}
: CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link) - ^ a b c d Ingram, John. C.L. (2007). Neurolinguistics: An Introduction to Spoken Language Processing and its Disorders. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. pp. 113–127.
- ^ a b Parker, Ellen M.; R.L. Diehl; K.R. Kluender (1986). "Trading Relations in Speech and Non-speech". Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics. 39 (2): 129–142. doi:10.3758/bf03211495. PMID 3725537.
- ^ Randy L. Diehl; Andrew J. Lotto; Lori L. Holt (2004). "Speech perception". Annual Review of Psychology. 55 (1): 149–179. doi:10.1146/annurev.psych.55.090902.142028. PMID 14744213. S2CID 937985.